(1) Introduction

Despite the substantial increase in the use of advanced and sophisticated statistical methods in linguistic research over the last decade (; ), one of the more controversial areas of statistical analysis (in general and in linguistics) is the analysis of Likert-type data. There has been a longstanding disagreement on which statistical methods are valid for the analysis of this type of data. The main point of disagreement is whether Likert-type data can be assumed to be interval data or not, i.e., whether the different points on the scale can be considered equidistant, or whether they should be considered ordinal data. This distinction is important since parametric statistics, like analysis of variance and mean calculation, are only applicable to continuous or interval data, while ordinal data requires the use of non-parametric statistics, such as the Kruskal -Wallis H test and calculation of the median. The selection of the type of statistic is not only integral to how the results are analysed, but also to the validity of the conclusions.

We contribute to this debate by comparing the results of three statistical tests to assess the degree of agreement between the different tests. Of special interest to us is the appropriateness and interpretability of the respective tests in a typical sociopragmatic investigation (; ). We therefore use data that was collected as part of a project that investigates various sociopragmatic aspects of swearwords (see section (3)).

Our analysis (section (4)) comprises a comparison of Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), Kruskal-Wallis H test (KW-H), and ordinal logistic regression (OLR) to ascertain how the results compare across various response and predictor variables. The aim is to answer, through a discussion of the results (section (5)), the following questions:

  1. Do the different statistical tests produce comparable results?
  2. Do different tests allow for different interpretations of the same data?
  3. Which statistical test is most appropriate and robust for interpreting Likert-type data in sociopragmatic linguistic investigations?

(2) Contextualisation and related work

The use of Likert and Likert-type items to measure and quantify typically qualitative data is widely used not only in sociopragmatic research (), but also in other areas in the Humanities and Social Sciences (; ). Likert () originally developed these Likert items such that a single qualitative attribute is measured multiple times and then averaged across the related questions for use with parametric statistical tests to remain valid. The use of individual Likert-type items has become increasingly widespread, and in turn caused longstanding disagreement between researchers on what the appropriate set of statistical tests for these Likert-type data should be (; ; ; ; ). The main point of contention is about the nature of the data: Is Likert-type data interval data, which allows for parametric and basic descriptive statistics (such as means and ANOVA) to be used in the analysis of the data; or is it ordinal data that should be analysed using those statistical tests specifically designed for ordinal data, which typically have substantially more complex assumptions that should be adhered to when performing statistical analysis.

The main argument for the use of parametric statistics is that parametric statistics are robust enough to handle data that is not necessarily normally distributed, and that is not continuous or interval data. Various power analysis investigations (; ; ) have been published where the veracity of this claim has been investigated, and in general confirmed the robustness of such tests on Likert-type data, especially when there is a large enough set of respondents. Van Hecke () however shows that ANOVA is less powerful than KW-H in the case of asymmetrical distributions and is likely to be more conservative in attributing significant effects. Other researchers (e.g., ) also argue that extending the scale beyond five options (to seven or nine), or that labelling only the extreme edges of the scale with nominal values while intermediary values are labelled numerically, further reduces the discrepancy between interval and ordinal data.

Specifically related to the domain of swearing, several studies have used Likert-type items to determine offensiveness, tabooness, self-perceived frequency of usage, and other related aspects of taboo language (e.g., , ; , ; ; ). Most of these studies only made use of ANOVA and descriptive statistics, with only one study using KW-H. A summary of some of the most prominent studies, including the kinds of statistical tests they used, is presented in Table 1.

Table 1

Summary of statistical tests used in previous studies on swearwords.


PUBLICATIONTASKASPECTS MEASUREDSCALE# USABLE RESPONDENTSSTATISTICAL TESTS

Beers Fägersten ()Word ratingsOffensiveness1–10 (Not offensive – Very offensive)60Descriptives (mean, mode, SD); One-way ANOVA test for significance

Beers Fägersten()Multiple choice questionsFrequency of swearingNever, Rarely, Sometimes, Often60Descriptives (percentages)

Likelihood judgementsFrequency of swearing0–100 (Not likely at all – Most likely possible); 1–9 (Never heard at all – Heard very frequently)53; 59Descriptives (mean)

Jay()Situation ratingsOffensiveness and tabooness1–9 (Not offensive/ obscene at all – Most offensive/ obscene word imaginable)52; 59; 90†Descriptives (mean, rank-order); ANOVA

Janschewitz ()Word ratingsPersonal use; familiarity; offensiveness; tabooness; valence; arousal; imageability1–9 (Positive/low –Negative/high)78One-way ANOVA; 2×5 mixed ANOVA; post hoc t test with Bonferroni corrected alphas

Dewaele()Multiple choice questionsPerception of emotional force1 = Does not feel strong; 2 = Little; 3 = Fairly; 4 = Strong; 5 = Very strong1039 + 50††Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA); Scheffe’ post-hoc test; linear regression analysis

Dewaele()Multiple choice questions; word ratingsFamiliarity with meaning; offensiveness; frequency0–5 (Very low – Very high)2347One-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test; Mann-Whitney test; Kruskal-Wallis H test

Note: † = Three different experiments. †† = Two modes of collection (online and paper-based).

Endresen and Janda () previously compared the results of five statistical models on acceptability judgments with a similar aim of determining which of the models are most appropriate to apply to Likert-type items. In their comparison of the models, they base their conclusions on the application of three predictor variables on the Likert-type acceptability ratings. They conclude that all five models are appropriate to a greater or lesser degree, and that the outcomes of both parametric and non-parametric tests provide comparable results, even though only two of their models significantly correlate the same predictor variables. They also crucially do not include the non-parametric equivalent of the ANOVA, KW-H test, in their experiments.

Although our study has similar aims to their investigation, there are several important differences. Firstly, the scope of their study was relatively limited in terms of the number of test cases based on predictor and outcome variables. This makes it difficult to draw definitive conclusions about the similarity and differences between the results of the different models, and consequently the applicability of the different models for Likert-type data. We address this by conducting a substantially larger set of experiments, 80 hypotheses tested in total, to get a clearer indication of how the results for the different statistics compare. Furthermore, the data in our experiments shows greater variance in the distributions of the outcomes, covering both highly skewed and relatively normal distributions. To simplify the comparisons between different statistics, we only consider a single predictor for each outcome, rather than the multiple predictors used in their study. Lastly, our application is in a different domain, i.e., tabooness ratings, rather than acceptability judgments, although we not expect the domain to have a substantial impact on the applicability of our results.

(2.1) Statistical tests

For purposes of this investigation, we will focus on three statistical tests often used in the analysis of Likert-type data, namely ANOVA, KW-H, and OLR. Each of the tests have different underlying assumptions: from the relatively simple assumptions for ANOVA, to the substantially more complex set of assumptions for OLR (for explanations of these assumptions, see most statistics textbooks, e.g., Verma and Abdel-Salam ()). Additionally, all these tests allow for potentially differing interpretations of the data and statistic results.

ANOVA is a parametric test used to determine if there are significant differences between two or more categorical groups (levels) by determining whether variance is only due to chance (within-group variation), or variance is due to chance and an effect, the predictor variable, (between-group variation). If the between-group variation is more dominant than the within-group variation, the effect is considered significant. If there are more than two levels, ANOVA does not specify which levels differ significantly; however, those can be calculated with post-hoc tests where the variance between each level is calculated, typically using Tukey’s HSD (honestly significant difference) (). ANOVA has two primary assumptions related to how the data fits the ANOVA model: (1) the normal distribution assumption, and (2) the homogeneity of variances assumption. As mentioned above, various power analysis studies have shown that ANOVA is robust against violation of the normality assumption if the levels have a sufficiently large set of respondents, typically when there are between two and nine levels with more than 15 respondents in each level.

KW-H (a.k.a. Kruskal-Wallis test by ranks, or one-way ANOVA on ranks ()) is a non-parametric counterpart of ANOVA, which does not make any assumptions about the distribution of the responses. It was originally designed to analyse ordinal, rather than interval or continuous data – especially where more than three levels are being compared. Although the test does not make direct assumptions about the distributions of data, the interpretation of the results does require visual inspection of the distribution of scores for each level of the predictor variable. If their distributions have the same shape, the test can interpret median values; alternatively, only statements about the distributions of each level can be made. As with ANOVA, KW-H does not directly identify which levels differ, but the Dunn post-hoc test (), which indicates which levels differ significantly from each other, is typically used to this end. Both Tukey’s HSD and the Dunn post-hoc test assess the significance of difference between pairs of group means. Unlike the omnibus tests (ANOVA and KW-H), these post hoc tests can indicate the specific groups where the means differ. It is important to note that although either ANOVA or KW-H may indicate a significant difference in means across all the levels, post-hoc tests may not indicate that there is a significant difference between any two groups.

The final statistical test that will be considered here is OLR, a parametric test that is specifically used when one has an ordinal response variable and one or more predictor variables that are continuous or categorical (). Like KW-H, it also does not require normally distributed data but does, however, require proportional odds between the levels (in addition to the assumption that two or more of the continuous independent variables are not highly correlated with each other – which is not applicable to our data). As a regression model, OLR is designed to make predictions of future behaviour, given a particular predictor variable on a particular response variable. The resultant model can be interpreted as an odds ratio: What are the odds that a member from level A will choose a higher or lower ordinal value than a member from level B? This is done by determining whether a more complex model (i.e., a model that includes the effect) is a better fit for the data than an intercept-only model. OLR can also consider multiple effects to improve the model, though that is not the focus of this study.

(3) Data contextualisation

To compare and assess these three statistical techniques, specifically to determine which of them is most efficient for interpreting typical sociopragmatic data, we use a small dataset from the project Swearing in South Africa: Multidisciplinary research on language taboos (). Broadly speaking, this project aims to get insight into a range of aspects related to the usage and perception of, and attitudes toward taboo language.

For example, in one of the subprojects, the aim is to empirically obtain offensiveness ratings for as many Afrikaans swearwords as practically possible through Short Word Surveys (SWS). This aim is comparable to the aims of studies done with L1 speakers of English (e.g., ; ; ; ; ), and Dutch (e.g., ), as well as LX speakers of English (e.g., , ), and other multilinguals (e.g., ). Although these kinds of offensiveness ratings are of academic interest to linguists, psychologists, and other researchers, our end goal is also more applied, namely, to develop a publicly accessible, evidence-based, online dataset of swearwords and their offensiveness ratings. This dataset can then be applied in various settings where the use of swearwords is either restricted by law or may have financial impact in a business setting.

This dataset is available () with a full description of the data and data collection procedure in Van Huyssteen et al. (). In short, participants register on the vloek.co.za website and provide sociodemographic information on 21 factors. The participants are then prompted to complete Short Word Surveys (SWS) where each participant rates their perceived attitudes for a particular swearword on a nine-point Likert scale for each of the following attitudinal dimensions: Production frequency, Perception frequency, Offensiveness (self), Tabooness (others), Emotionality, Conspicuousness, Familiarity, and Sex of referent.

In South Africa, it is by law for the task of the Film and Publication Board (FPB) to provide content and age advisories for films, computer games, and certain publications that are released/published in South Africa. One of the criteria relates to “strong language”, which is defined as “crude words, threats, abuse, profanity or language that amounts to prejudice” (). Such strong language should be categorised as “of a mild, moderate, strong or very strong impact”. However, this offensiveness scale is not operationalised (not even by means of examples), and authors, publishers, film makers, and other content creators are therefore left without any practical guidelines.

For example, a publisher may want to include a potentially taboo word in the title of a novel but would like to determine how the FPB might respond to such a title. Instead of doing their own (expensive and time-consuming) market research, they could rather consult said dataset as a dashboard, adjust the variables to fit their target audience (e.g., in terms of age, gender, level of qualification, income group, religious views, etc.), and get results on the perception of the word, and the impact it might have on that specific audience. This could then be presented as arguments in an appeal against a decision of the FPB.

However, for more sophisticated and reliable interpretations using sociodemographic variables, the project needs an appropriate and reliable statistical technique to analyse and report on these ratings in ways that are not only clear and informative, but also robust enough to implement as part of an automated workflow (i.e., an implementation that does not require a human in the loop).

The current study focuses only on four attitudinal dimensions (Production frequency; Perception frequency; Offensiveness; and Tabooness) of a selection of four swearwords (feeks; piele; moffie; jissis – see below) using five sociodemographic factors (age group; sex group; religious view; political view; world view). The four words were chosen to represent a variety of morphosyntactic, semantic, and pragmatic aspects (marked in bold below) of swearwords, so that the findings of this article should be applicable to the analysis of a variety of other swearwords as well. The four sources of Tabooness and the four chosen words are:

  • Misogyny: feeks (‘harridan’) is a rather old-fashioned word, used as an epithet for a strong-willed woman;
  • Sexuality: piele (‘penises’; ‘fucking A’) is a noun, denoting more than one penis; however, in the context asked in the SWS, it is used as an adjective or interjection to indicate that something is good, fantastic, perfect;
  • Bigotry: moffie (‘faggot’) is a slur for a gay man, but as was the case for the English word gay, moffie is in the process of being reappropriated by the gay community; and
  • Blasphemy: jissis (‘jeezuz’) is a deformation of the religious name Jesus, and it is mostly used as an interjection.

The four attitudinal dimensions considered in this study primarily relate to the usage of and attitude to the swearwords. Production refers to how often the respondent says or writes a swearword, while Perception indicates how often the respondent hears or reads a swearword. Offensiveness relates to how offensive the respondent finds a word themselves, and Tabooness equates to how offensive the respondent thinks other people perceive the swearword to be. In the subsequent tests, the attitudinal dimensions are considered as the response variables, while sociodemographic factors act as predictor variables. Based on the responses from between 147 and 179 participants (see below), the median values of the four response variables for each of the four words are summarised in Table 2.

Table 2

Median Likert scale (1–9) values of the four response variables for the four swearwords.


WORDPRODUCTIONPERCEPTIONOFFENSIVENESSTABOONESS

feeks3523

piele1357

moffie2477

jissis1498

The selection of attitudinal dimensions and sociodemographic factors are also done in order to cover a range of predictor and response variables that are aligned closely with the source of tabooness of the word under consideration, viz.:

  1. Misogyny: We expect words like feeks, which might be use pejoratively in reference to women, to be more offensive to female respondents. This expectation is based on the assumption that terms of abuse will be more abusive (and hence offensive and perceived as taboo) to the abusee, than to the abuser. Furthermore, since the word is old-fashioned, one can expect that younger respondents will use the word less often and find it less offensive.
  2. Sexuality: Numerous studies have indicated that men use strong swearwords much more often than women (see for a brief summary of findings). We therefore hypothesise that male respondents will report that they use and encounter words like piele more often than female respondents. It is also expected that older men produce and perceive these types of words more often than younger men.
  3. Bigotry: Since homophobia is a sociological phenomenon where hegemonic masculinity is purported as superior to alternate masculinities (see, among others, , ch. 2), we expect that a word like moffie will be perceived as less offensive by older, conservative male respondents (i.e., people representing hegemonic masculinity).
  4. Blasphemy: It is expected a priori that blasphemous words will be more offensive to more religious and conservative people.

Although not all registered users completed the SWSs for all four words (see Section (4)), the total number of respondents for each of the words constitute a relatively large sample, ranging between 147 and 179 participants. Table 3 summarises the distribution of respondents for each of the four words across sociodemographic factor levels.

Table 3

Sociodemographic summary of respondents for the four swearwords.


SOCIODEMOGRAPHIC FACTORLEVEL FEEKS (n= 147) PIELE (n= 167) MOFFIE (n= 179) JISSIS (n= 152)

n (%)

Age group18–3955 (37.41)65 (38.92)67 (37.43)63 (41.45)

40–5962 (42.18)66 (39.52)76 (42.46)59 (38.82)

60+30 (20.41)36 (21.56)36 (20.11)30 (19.74)

SexMale55 (37.41)63 (37.72)73 (40.78)59 (38.82)

Female92 (62.59)104 (62.28)106 (59.22)93 (61.18)

Religious viewsVery religious30 (20.41)35 (20.96)34 (18.99)33 (21.71)

Religious56 (38.10)65 (38.92)65 (36.31)58 (38.16)

Moderate19 (12.93)23 (13.77)26 (14.53)24 (15.79)

Not really18 (12.24)18 (10.78)21 (11.73)17 (11.18)

Not at all24 (16.33)26 (15.57)33 (18.44)20 (13.16)

Political viewsVery Conservative2 (1.36)2 (1.20)2 (1.12)4 (2.63)

Conservative11 (7.48)14 (8.38)13 (7.26)12 (7.89)

Moderate61 (41.50)75 (44.91)75 (41.90)69 (45.39)

Liberal34 (23.13)39 (23.35)43 (24.02)36 (23.68)

Very liberal39 (26.53)37 (22.16)46 (25.70)31 (20.39)

World viewVery Conservative2 (1.36)2 (1.20)2 (1.12)2 (1.32)

Conservative17 (11.56)22 (13.17)19 (10.61)19 (12.50)

Moderate37 (25.17)44 (26.35)44 (24.58)46 (30.26)

Liberal34 (23.13)44 (26.35)43 (24.02)36 (23.68)

Very liberal57 (38.78)55 (32.93)71 (39.66)49 (32.24)

Two further details regarding the respondents are relevant to our analyses:

  1. Respondents who did not choose any of the levels listed above (e.g., who chose to not answer a question), were excluded from this analysis, since respondents who do not specify one of the levels could potentially belong to any of the levels. The inclusion of these respondents would violate the independence assumption underpinning all of the statistical methods under consideration, and potentially invalidate the reported results.
  2. For both the political and world view factors, only two respondents identified as Very Conservative, which are too few respondents to make valid statistical inferences. In all of the subsequent hypothesis testing, the Very Conservative level is excluded from consideration.

(4) Methodology

For the purpose of comparing the different tests, each of the three tests is applied to all of the combinations of words, response and predictor variables. In total, 80 different hypotheses are tested (i.e., five sociodemographic factors x four attitudinal dimensions x four words) using the three tests. The aim of these hypothesis tests is not to identify significant effects, but rather to compare the hypotheses that are considered significant by each of the statistics to ascertain whether the identified predictors and response variables are different. Significance is considered at the 95% confidence level.

For ANOVA, eight of the hypotheses showed a significant effect for the predictor variable on the response variable (see Table 4). Both KW-H and OLR also showed significant effects between levels for these eight hypotheses. An analysis of the post-hoc tests for both ANOVA and KW-H identified the same levels where statistically significant effects are observed.

Table 4

Significant ANOVA results.


WORDRESPONSEPREDICTORnMEDIANANOVA p KW-H p OLR p

feeksProductionAge1473.010.004.005

feeksOffensivenessSex1472.038.009N/A

pielePerceptionWorld view165†3.022.009.001

pieleOffensivenessAge1675.001.001.016

pieleOffensivenessSex1675<.0005<.0005<.0005

pieleOffensivenessPolitical view165†5.038.028.007

pieleOffensivenessWorld view165†5.013.012.001

pieleTaboonessSex1677.007.005.004

Note: N/A = Violation of the assumption of proportional odds for OLR; † = Respondents from “Very conservative” level excluded since there were fewer than five respondents.

In addition to the eight hypotheses with significant effects identified by ANOVA, KW-H identified a further 17 significant effects for a total of 25 effects (see Table 5). There is also substantial overlap between these hypotheses and the OLR results for the same hypotheses, with only a single instance where OLR does not identify a significant effect, namely piele | Production | Age (marked in bold in the last column in Table 5). However, there are six hypotheses where the assumption of proportional odds for OLR is violated (marked “N/A” in the last column in Table 5).

Table 5

Significant KW-H results.


WORDRESPONSEPREDICTORnMEDIANANOVA p KW-H p OLR p

pieleProductionAge1671.088.043.251

pieleProductionSex1671N/A<.0005<.0005

pieleProductionPolitical view165†1.069.008N/A

pieleProductionWorld view165†1N/A.0134.001

pielePerceptionPolitical view165†3.074.034N/A

pieleTaboonessAge1677.058.004.015

moffieProductionSex1792N/A.008.002

moffieOffensivenessSex1797N/A.004.002

jissisProductionSex1521N/A.001.002

jissisProductionPolitical view148†1N/A.001N/A

jissisProductionReligious view1521N/A<.0005<.0005

jissisProductionWorld view150†1N/A<.0005N/A

jissisOffensivenessSex1529.0526.044.031

jissisOffensivenessPolitical view148†9N/A.001N/A

jissisOffensivenessReligious view1529N/A<.0005N/A

jissisOffensivenessWorld view150†9N/A<.0005<.0005

jissisTaboonessReligious view1528.054.016.010

Note: N/A = Violation of assumption of equal variance for ANOVA, or assumption of proportional odds for OLR; † = Respondents from “Very conservative” level excluded since there were fewer than five respondents.

Note that in most of the 17 hypotheses identified by KW-H, the assumption of homoscedasticity for ANOVA is violated (marked “N/A” in the ANOVA column in Table 5). Although the p values for ANOVA that do not violate this assumption would be significant at the 90% level, they are not significant at the more accepTable 95% level.

It is also noticeable that KW-H seems to identify hypotheses with significant effects even with highly skewed distributions, i.e., where median values are at the very extreme ends of the Likert scale. In contrast, ANOVA mainly identifies hypotheses where the median is closer to the centre of the scale, although this is not always the case (e.g., see moffie | Offensiveness | Sex), as discussed in more detail in the following section.

Lastly, in addition to the hypotheses identified as significant by ANOVA and KW-H, three additional effects are identified by OLR only (see Table 6). OLR therefore identified a total of 20 effects (i.e., seven in Table 4, ten in Table 5, and three in Table 6). In all these cases, the proportional odds assumption for OLR has not been violated.

Table 6

Significant OLR results.


WORDRESPONSEPREDICTORnMEDIANANOVA p KW-H p OLR p

pielePerceptionReligious view1673.207.125.040

pieleOffensivenessReligious view1675.132.086.016

moffiePerceptionPolitical view1774.338.304.030

(5) Results and discussion

Our first research question (see section (1)) was whether the different statistical tests produce comparable results. From the results presented in the previous section, the two tests that were designed specifically to work with ordinal data (KW-H and OLR) not only identified the same eight significant hypotheses as ANOVA, but also additional significant effects: KW-H identified a total of 25 hypotheses and OLR 20. Given the fact that ANOVA is not necessarily simpler to run or interpret than KW-H – both require post-hoc tests to determine the specific levels between which significant effects are observed – there does not seem to be a good reason for using a parametric test (ANOVA) for Likert-type data, especially when the value distributions are highly skewed.

When choosing between KW-H and OLR, there seems to be a high degree of agreement between them when considering a single predictor variable. On the downside for OLR though, is that a substantial number of hypotheses are not permissible when considering the OLR assumption of proportional odds, along with the fact that the test is substantially more complex to apply and interpret. It would therefore seem advisable to use KW-H for Likert-type data of this kind. One does however also need to consider how the results for each of these tests can be interpreted and how the interpretation possibilities address the fundamental aims of a particular study.

The second research question concerned the descriptive power of the respective tests and the interpretation possibilities for each. Both ANOVA and KW-H only indicate that there are statistically significant differences between one or more of the levels under consideration, and both require a post-hoc test to indicate which levels differ significantly. It is possible for an overall test to indicate that there are statistically significant differences between levels, but that the post-hoc test does not show a significant difference between any two levels. In the data used in this study, however, there are at least two levels that show significant differences for all of the tests identified by either the ANOVA or KW-H.

As an example of the results of these post-hoc tests, Tables 7 and 8 show the post-hoc test results when comparing the different individual Age levels for the Production of the word feeks. Both post-hoc tests indicate that there is statistically significant difference between Production for the 18–39 and 40–59 age levels, while only the Dunn test indicates that there is a difference between 18–39 and 60+ levels. These tests still don’t provide further insight into the nature of the difference, i.e., whether people between 18 and 39 produce feeks more or less often. Further analysis is therefore required.

Table 7

Dunn’s post-hoc test results for Production of the word feeks.


AGE GROUP18–3940–5960+

18–391.000.006.037

40–59.0061.0001.000

60+.0371.0001.000

Table 8

Tukey’s HSD test results for Production of the word feeks.


AGE GROUP18–3940–5960+

18–391.000.014.053

40–59.0141.000.999

60+.053.9991.000

As part of the review of KW-H, it is necessary to do a visual inspection of the box plots of the different levels in order to determine whether the distributions of the various levels are the same (or at least very similar), since this determines how the results can be interpreted. If the distributions are similar, one can make statements about the median of the respective level, for example the median of 18–39 level is one point lower than that of the 40–59 level. If the distributions are not similar, one can only make statements about the most likely response of members of a particular level, for example a randomly selected member of the 18–39 group is more likely to assign a lower Production value than a member from the 40–59 group. Visual inspection of the box plot in Figure 1 shows that the distribution of the three Age levels is not similar enough to adhere to KW-H assumption and therefore we can only interpret the results in terms of the distribution, not the median. In this case, a randomly selected member of the 18–39 group will assign a lower Production value than either the 40–59 or 60+ groups statistically significantly.

Figure 1 

Box plot of different Age levels for the Production of feeks.

OLR models allow for a slightly different interpretation of the results, as the test is specifically designed to make predictions of expected future behaviour in the parameter estimates. OLR gives a representation of the significance (p < 0.05) of the predictor variable (e.g., Age) in relation to a response variable (e.g., Production) for a reference level (e.g., 18–39), as well as an odds-ratio which indicates how much more likely one level is to assign a higher or lower value than the reference level. Tables 9 and 10 provide an overview of the results for two levels (40–59 and 60+) compared to the reference level (18–39) when considering the Production of feeks with Age as the predictor variable.

Table 9

OLR significance values for the Production of feeks.


AGE GROUP18–3940–5960+

18–391.000.001.008

40–59.0011.000.983

60+.008.9831.000

Table 10

OLR odds ratio values for the Production of feeks.


AGE GROUP18–3940–5960+

18–391.000.333.335

40–59.3331.0001.008

60+.3351.0081.000

These results indicate that a person from the 18–39 group is significantly more likely to assign a lower value (odds ratio < 1) than people from either the 40–59 (p = .001) and 60+ (p = .008) levels at ratios of 3.0 (1/.333) and 2.99 (1/.335) to one. These ratios should be interpreted as follows: three people in the 18–39 age group will assign a lower value for each person in the 18–39 level that assigns a higher value. Between the 40–59 and 60+ there is not a significant difference (p = .983) as substantiated by the odds ratio of 1.008, which is almost identical.

The answer to the last research question (which statistical test is most appropriate and robust for interpreting the data?) serves as conclusion of the article.

(6) Implications and Applications

There has been longstanding disagreement within the statistical research community on the appropriate statistical test to apply to Likert-type data. Some proponents of the more straightforward ANOVA argue that the test is more easily interpretable and is sufficiently robust to deal with data that violate the main assumptions of the test, viz. following a Gaussian distribution and constituting interval data (; ). Opponents argue that the violation of the assumptions is serious enough to cast doubt on the results and conclusions drawn from the parametric tests, and that one should rather use non-parametric tests specifically designed for ordinal data, such as KW-H or the substantially more complex parametric OLR.

In this work, we show that although there is some overlap in the results between the different statistical tests when analysing Likert-type data, KW-H in general performs best when analysing this type of data, especially when the responses are heavily skewed, as is the case of controversial subjects such as offensive language. Beyond the fact that KW-H validates a substantially larger group of effects, which in and of itself does not indicate a superior test statistic, the test shows almost identical behaviour to ANOVA where ANOVA does indicate statistical significance. Further analysis of KW-H results indicate that those effects identified, but not identified by ANOVA, do in fact show effects that indicate a significant difference between levels, and is corroborated by OLR. Since ANOVA and KW-H are similarly interpretable, the results indicate that it should be preferable to use KW-H, rather than ANOVA, when working with Likert-type data.

OLR is a substantially more complex statistical model to generate given the required assumptions associated with the test. The results of the OLR models largely agree with KW-H, and apart from the fact that the test allows for a different interpretation model, the odds-ratio, which gives a more powerful description of the differences between levels, does not provide additional qualities which would necessitate its use over that of KW-H, unless one is specifically interested in reviewing multiple predictor variables, for example combining Age and Gender, in which case this would be the most appropriate test.